The original GPS: How we remember what happened where

compass

Editor's Introduction

Neural activity in human hippocampal formation reveals the spatial context of retrieved memories

annotated by

Do you remember when you first met your childhood best friend? You might remember that you sat next to each other in class or in the lunchroom, or that you shared an interest in the same hobby, like riding bikes. If you went to the park where you used to play as a child, you might recall your old bike. How do we form memories for places and objects? What part of the brain controls those memories, and why does the park where you played as a child remind you of your old bicycle? In this study, the authors measured the activity of brain cells (neurons) in specific areas of the brain known to control memory while participants played a memory video game.

Paper Details

Original title
Neural activity in human hippocampal formation reveals the spatial context of retrieved memories
Original publication date
Reference
Vol. 342 no. 6162 pp. 1111-1114
Issue name
Science
DOI
10.1126/science.1244056

Abstract

In many species, spatial navigation is supported by a network of place cells that exhibit increased firing whenever an animal is in a certain region of an environment. Does this neural representation of location form part of the spatiotemporal context into which episodic memories are encoded? We recorded medial temporal lobe neuronal activity as epilepsy patients performed a hybrid spatial and episodic memory task. We identified place-responsive cells active during virtual navigation and then asked whether the same cells activated during the subsequent recall of navigation-related memories without actual navigation. Place-responsive cell activity was reinstated during episodic memory retrieval. Neuronal firing during the retrieval of each memory was similar to the activity that represented the locations in the environment where the memory was initially encoded.

Report

When one encounters an old friend and remembers the time they last met, often the place of meeting and surrounding circumstances come to mind. This is the hallmark of episodic memory: the capacity to store and later retrieve memories that are bound to a particular place and time (1). Theories of episodic memory posit that the brain supports this ability by continually maintaining an updated representation of the current spatiotemporal context, which is a neural representation of space, time, and other aspects of one’s current cognitive milieu (2). When the brain forms a new episodic memory, these theories predict that the content of the experience becomes associated with the current spatial and temporal context. When the memory is retrieved, this prior context is partially reinstated, focusing one’s thoughts on the time and place of the remembered episode. This reinstatement not only provides the phenomenological experience of remembering, but also helps to cue other memories experienced within the same or related contexts.

Although it is well established that the hippocampus and surrounding medial-temporal-lobe (MTL) structures play a central role in the formation and retrieval of context-mediated memories (35), we know far less about how these memory processes manifest in the activities of individual MTL neurons. Much of what is known about the neural coding properties of hippocampal and MTL neurons comes from studies of rodent spatial navigation, where individual neurons respond preferentially at specific locations within a given contextually defined spatial environment (67). Similar neuronal responses have also been identified in the human hippocampus during virtual spatial navigation (89). The context-dependent firing of these neurons (10, 11) and their dependence on the animal’s goal state or past history of experienced cues (12, 13) have led some to speculate that the neural representation of space in the hippocampus is part of a broader network of neurons that encode episodic memories more generally (1417). This hypothesis suggests that the same neural structures and computations that enable the learning of a spatial layout via place-cell activity also facilitate the encoding of episodic memories. However, according to a prominent alternative account, the spatial coding functions of the hippocampus are part of a context module that operates independently of the computations that encode the content of a memory (18,19).

We designed a virtual-reality memory game in which participants played the role of a delivery person, driving through a virtual town and delivering items to stores. Our participants were patients with drug-resistant epilepsy who were implanted with depth electrodes to localize the focus of their seizures and to map cognitive function in surrounding healthy tissue. In an initial phase of the game, participants explored the town using a computer controller to navigate from store to store as they attempted to learn the layout of the environment illustrated inFig. 1A. After this initial familiarization phase, during which participants visited each store twice, a series of “delivery days” began. On each delivery day, participants were instructed to travel from store to store, visiting 13 randomly chosen stores (of the 16 total) in a randomly determined order. Upon their arrival at each store, participants were presented with an item [either visually for 2 s for participants one to five or aurally for participants six and seven (20)]. Upon arrival at the final (13th) store, no item was presented. Instead, the screen went black and participants were prompted to vocally recall as many of the 12 delivered items as they could remember in any order (participants recalled 5.2 items, on average). After being given 90 s for free recall, participants could advance to a new delivery day, in which they would deliver a distinct but randomly determined set of items to a random sequence of 13 stores and then attempt to recall the new set of items. Consistent with earlier work (21), participants exhibited a significant (P = 0.008) tendency to consecutively recall items delivered to more spatially proximate locations (see supplementary text).

vf1.jpg

Fig. 1.  The behavioral task.  (A) Overhead map of the virtual environment. Red rectangles, store locations; blue squares, locations of nonstore buildings; green areas, grass and trees; small dark blue, brown, and yellow boxes; mailboxes, benches, and street lights. (B) An example storefront that a participant might encounter. (Translation of text at top left: “Please find the bakery.”) (C) The presentation of an item (a zucchini) upon arrival at the target store (bakery). (Translation of text at top left: “Successfully delivered: a zucchini.”) (D) The initiation of the recall period, as indicated by a black screen with asterisks.

Description of the virtual tasks being used for the study  

Panel A depicts the layout of the apartment complex in the virtual tour, including store locations, mailboxes, streets, and grassy areas.

Panel B depicts what the typical store locations look like in the virtual task.

Panel C shows the presentation of an object once the subject has come to the target location. 

Panel D shows a screen that indicates the “recall” period of the task.

What questions will the experimenters answer using the virtual task?   

Share your thoughts with your classmates. 

Virtual navigation task

See this video of a virtual navigation task.

Morris water task (MWT)

See this video of the Morris water task (MWT), which is used to study spatial navigation in rodents.

We first sought to identify patterns of neuronal activity that represented participants’ location within the virtual town. We identified place-responsive cells as the neurons that exhibited significantly increased firing at a particular location in the virtual environment (20). Figure 2A depicts the activity of one example place-responsive cell, which increased its firing rate when the participant was positioned at a location on the left side of the virtual environment and facing north. The majority of the identified place-responsive cells were direction-dependent (72%) and did not exhibit significant place fields when direction of traversal was not taken into account. This is similar to earlier findings of directionally oriented place cells in environments with clearly defined routes, in contrast to open environments, where omnidirectional place cells are prevalent (822). These directionally oriented place cells were not generally responsive to place-invariant view information (see supplementary text). Figure 2B shows the firing rate of a place-responsive cell from the entorhinal cortex, which activated at a location in the south part of the environment during eastward movements. In total, we identified 95 place-responsive cells, making up 25.6% of all observed neurons. There were significant numbers of place-responsive cells in the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and amygdala and in anterior MTL regions of ambiguous localization (20) (binomial test with P < 0.01 for each region) (Fig. 2C and tables S1 and S2).

vf2.jpg

Fig. 2.  Place-responsive cells.  (A) Firing-rate map for a cell responsive to northward traversals located in participant six’s hippocampus, shown separately for each cardinal direction. Gray represents all areas traversed by the participant, regardless of the direction of travel. (B) A cell responsive to eastward traversals recorded from participant one’s entorhinal cortex. (C) Regional distribution of place-responsive cells in the entire data set of 371 single units (H, hippocampus; A, amygdala; EC, entorhinal cortex; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; Ant, anterior medial temporal lobe). The red dashed line indicates the false-positive rate of 5%. Asterisks denote brain regions where the number of place-responsive cells significantly exceeded chance levels.

Question being asked

Can the authors identify patterns of neuronal firing that represent the participants’ location while navigating the virtual task?

Results

Firing rates of 72% of the place cells recorded depended on the direction in which the participant was traveling during virtual navigation.

For example, some place cells in the entorhinal cortex were only responsive when the participant was in the south region of the environment and moving east.

In figure 2, panel C, the graph shows which brain regions contained “place-responsive” cells. The bars represent the percentage of the total number of recorded cells (371) within each brain region that are “place-responsive.”

Methods

Single cell recordings from the entorhinal cortex

Statistical methods for comparison of groups: binomial test

Live place cell recordings

Watch this video to see live place cell recordings from a rat.

Conclusions

The majority of the place-responsive cells measured were only responsive when direction was taken into account. Of all the cells from which recordings were monitored, about 25% were in the hippocampus and surrounding hippocampal region.

To determine whether spontaneous retrieval of items during free recall reinstated the spatial context associated with the item’s encoding, we calculated the neural similarity between ensemble place-responsive cell activity during navigation and during item retrieval [see (20) and fig. S1 for further details]. We partitioned the environment into three regions for each recalled item: regions close to the delivery location, regions of intermediate distance, and regions that were far from the delivery location. We then asked whether the ensemble place-cell activity at the time of retrieval was more similar to navigational epochs that were closer to the delivery location. A high degree of similarity would indicate the reinstatement of the spatial context associated with the item. To protect against potential confounding between item and spatial context, we excluded navigational epochs surrounding the delivery of an item.

We found significant spatial context reinstatement surrounding the time of item vocalization (time course illustrated in Fig. 3A). The level of neural similarity between recall activity and navigation activity was ordered such that areas of the environment near an item’s encoding location exhibited the highest similarity scores, intermediate spatial distances exhibited middling similarity scores, and far spatial distances exhibited the lowest similarity scores (this effect was strongest over the interval of –300 to 700 ms, illustrated in Fig. 3B). An analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated a significant effect of distance on the level of neural similarity (F2,300 = 7.6, P < 0.001). Performing this latter analysis across participants rather than recall events revealed that neural similarity within the near distance bin was significantly greater than that within the far distance bin (Fig. 3C) [t(5) = 4.0, P = 0.009].

vf3.jpg

Fig. 3.  Spatial context reinstatement.  (A) Time courses of neural similarity between ensemble place-cell activity during navigation and during item recall are shown for near, middle, and far spatial distance bins. Time courses, shown relative to recall onset, were computed in overlapping 500-ms windows (x values indicate the center of the window). Similarity is defined as the cosine of the angle between ensemble activity during recall and navigation, normalized as a rank score (20). Shaded regions indicate SEM across recalled items. The horizontal bar indicates statistically significant time points, as determined by ANOVA with a false-discovery rate–adjusted significance threshold of 0.017. The vertical dotted line at 0 ms denotes the onset of the vocalization. (B) Average neural similarity for near, middle, and far spatial distance bins is shown for the time period of –300 to 700 ms relative to recall onset. Error bars indicate SEM across recalled items. (C) Neural similarity for near and far spatial distance bins for each of the included participants (thin colored lines) and the participant average (thick black line) is shown for the time period of –300 to 700 ms relative to recall onset. Error bars indicate SEM across participants.

Question being asked  

The authors asked if firing patterns of groups of neurons (ensemble place cells) during navigation (when an object is first encountered) are similar to firing patterns of ensemble place cells during recall? The authors also asked if ensemble place cell firing patterns during recall were dependent on distance from the location in which the object was presented.

Panel A

Panel A shows the activation of ensemble place-cells during recall when the participant was near, in the middle, or distant from the object’s location.

Panel B and C

Panel B and C show that during recall, ensemble place cell firing is less similar to initial firing patterns as the participant navigates farther from the location in which the object was first encountered.

Why is it important to record the activity of the cells before recall of an item?  

Discuss your thoughts with your classmates

Conclusions
As you can see from the graphs in panel B and C, as the participant moves farther away from the location where an object was first encountered, neural activation patterns during recall of the object become less similar. 

During the spontaneous recall of an item, place-responsive cells exhibited firing patterns similar to those shown during exploration of the region of the town where the item was previously delivered. Thus, recalling an episodic memory involves recovery of its spatial context, as seen in the activity of place-responsive cells in the human hippocampal formation and surrounding MTL regions. If the item delivery occurred in or near a cell’s place field, characterized by a firing rate that is significantly higher than the baseline level, then recalling the item should also produce an increase in firing rate. We calculated the firing rate of place-responsive cells when participants were navigating inside and outside of each cell’s place field, as well as the firing rate when participants recalled items that were presented near to or far from each cell’s respective place field (Fig. 4) [see (20) and fig. S2 for further details]. The average in-field firing rate (3.8 Hz) was substantially higher than the out-of-field firing rate [1.9 Hz;t(32) = 5.9, P < 10–5]. The average firing rate during the recall of items presented near a place field was 2.2 Hz, which was significantly higher than the 1.8-Hz firing rate during recall of items presented far from a place field [t(32) = 2.2, P = 0.03].

vf4.jpg

Fig. 4.  Place-responsive cell activity during navigation and recall.  (Left) Navigation. “In Field” indicates the average place-responsive cell firing rate when navigating within a cell’s place field, whereas “Out Field” indicates the average place-responsive cell firing rate at locations outside of a place field (*P < 10–5). (Right) Recall. “Near” indicates the average place-responsive cell firing rate in the time period from 1.5 s before to 1 s after recall onset of items that were initially presented in or close to the center of a place field. In contrast, “Far” represents the average place-responsive cell firing rate in the same time window for recall of items that were initially presented far from the center of a place field (*P = 0.03). Error bars indicate SEM.

Question being asked

Single cell recording shows that place-responsive cells exhibit increased firing rates during navigation with in a specific “place field.” The authors asked if place-responsive cells (which exhibit increased firing rates in specific place fields) show the same increase in firing rate during recall.

Results

The graph depicts the firing rate of “place cells” during navigation. The left side of the graph shows a significant increase in the firing rate of place cells when navigating within the cells’ place field versus when navigating outside the cells’ place field. The right side of the graph depicts the firing rate of place cells during recall of objects that were presented either “near” or “far” from the place field of the group of cells.

Single cell recording of place cells

Learn more about place fields and single cell recording of place cells here.

Morris water task (MWT) Place cell recordings

See a video of place cell recordings in a rat.

Conclusions

As you can see from the graph, during the recall phase, place cells fire more rapidly when the object presented was near the place field as compared with when the object presented was farther from the place field of the cells.

Unlike traditional list-recall studies of episodic memory, in which items unfold only in time, the present experiment provided a distinct spatial context for each item. This allowed us to leverage the spatial-coding properties of hippocampal neurons in the study of the neural basis of episodic recollection. Spatially sensitive neural activity in the hippocampal formation became reactivated during episodic retrieval, when no visual cues were present. At the time of recall, participants simply vocalized the names of the delivered items in the order in which they came to mind, yet the neurons responsive to spatial information reactivated during the time just before and during vocalization. This reactivation implies that each experienced item is bound to its spatial context, which in turn may be reinstated when the item comes to mind during recall.

Because human neural recordings are rarely possible, little is known about the neural substrates of spontaneous verbal recall. Nonetheless, several recent studies have established the general phenomena of content reinstatement, whereby the attributes of an item at encoding become reinstated just before recall. This has been shown for human hippocampal neurons that are selective for taxonomic categories, or possibly individual items (23), and also for distributed patterns of intracranial electroencephalography and hemodynamic activity (2425). Reinstatement is not specific to an individual item but also activates neighboring items, as would be expected if those neighboring items provide an abstract temporal context for the recalled item (2627). Such a temporal context signal may be reflected in the recent discovery of individual neurons in the rodent hippocampus that appear to encode the relative times of behaviorally important events (2829).

Our finding that spontaneous recall of an item reactivates its spatial context provides direct neural evidence for theories of episodic memory that postulate context reinstatement as the basis for recollection (230). This result also implies that the spatial coding identified with the hippocampal place-cell system is part of a more general engine of episodic memory in which items become associated with their spatiotemporal contexts, and retrieval of items reinstates those contexts to help cue other context-appropriate memories.

Supplementary Materials

www.sciencemag.org/content/342/6162/1111/suppl/DC1

Materials and Methods

Supplementary Text

Figs. S1 to S3

Tables S1 and S2

References

References and Notes

  1. E. Tulving, Elements of Episodic Memory (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1983).

  2. S. M. Polyn, M. J. Kahana, Memory search and the neural representation of context. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 24–30 (2008).

  3. W. B. Scoville, B. Milner, Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 11–21 (1957).

  4. H. Eichenbaum, Hippocampus: Cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron 44, 109–120 (2004).

  5. L. Davachi, Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 693–700 (2006).

  6. J. O’Keefe, L. Nadel, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1978).

  7. B. L. McNaughton, F. P. Battaglia, O. Jensen, E. I. Moser, M.-B. Moser, Path integration and the neural basis of the “cognitive map”. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 663–678 (2006).

  8. D. Ekstrom, M. J. Kahana, J. B. Caplan, T. A. Fields, E. A. Isham, E. L. Newman, I. Fried, Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature 425, 184–188 (2003).

  9. J. Jacobs, M. J. Kahana, A. D. Ekstrom, M. V. Mollison, I. Fried, A sense of direction in human entorhinal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 6487–6492 (2010).

  10. R. U. Muller, J. L. Kubie, The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. J. Neurosci. 7, 1951–1968 (1987).

  11. S. Leutgeb, J. K. Leutgeb, M. B. Moser, E. I. Moser, Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 738–746 (2005).

  12. E. R. Wood, P. A. Dudchenko, R. J. Robitsek, H. Eichenbaum, Hippocampal neurons encode information about different types of memory episodes occurring in the same location. Neuron 27, 623–633 (2000).

  13. J. Ferbinteanu, M. L. Shapiro, Prospective and retrospective memory coding in the hippocampus. Neuron 40, 1227–1239 (2003).

  14. H. Eichenbaum, P. Dudchenko, E. Wood, M. Shapiro, H. Tanila, The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: Is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 23, 209–226 (1999).

  15. G. Buzsáki, Theta rhythm of navigation: Link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 15, 827–840 (2005).

  16. M. W. Howard, M. S. Fotedar, A. V. Datey, M. E. Hasselmo, The temporal context model in spatial navigation and relational learning: Toward a common explanation of medial temporal lobe function across domains. Phys. Rev. 112, 75–116 (2005).

  17. G. Buzsáki, E. I. Moser, Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 130–138 (2013).

  18. L. Davachi, J. P. Mitchell, A. D. Wagner, Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories.

  19. E. L. Hargreaves, G. Rao, I. Lee, J. J. Knierim, Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal hippocampus. Science 308, 1792–1794 (2005).

  20. Materials and methods are available as supplementary materials on Science Online.

  21. J. F. Miller, E. M. Lazarus, S. M. Polyn, M. J. Kahana, Spatial clustering during memory search. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 39, 773–781 (2013).

  22. R. U. Muller, E. Bostock, J. S. Taube, J. L. Kubie, On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. J. Neurosci. 14, 7235–7251 (1994).

  23. H. Gelbard-Sagiv, R. Mukamel, M. Harel, R. Malach, I. Fried, Internally generated reactivation of single neurons in human hippocampus during free recall. Science 322, 96–101 (2008).

  24. J. R. Manning, M. R. Sperling, A. Sharan, E. A. Rosenberg, M. J. Kahana, Spontaneously reactivated patterns in frontal and temporal lobe predict semantic clustering during memory search. J. Neurosci. 32, 8871–8878 (2012).

  25. S. M. Polyn, V. S. Natu, J. D. Cohen, K. A. Norman, Category-specific cortical activity precedes retrieval during memory search. Science 310, 1963–1966 (2005).

  26. J. R. Manning, S. M. Polyn, G. H. Baltuch, B. Litt, M. J. Kahana, Oscillatory patterns in temporal lobe reveal context reinstatement during memory search. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 12893–12897 (2011).

  27. M. W. Howard, I. V. Viskontas, K. H. Shankar, I. Fried, Ensembles of human MTL neurons “jump back in time” in response to a repeated stimulus. Hippocampus 22, 1833–1847 (2012).

  28. E. Pastalkova, V. Itskov, A. Amarasingham, G. Buzsáki, Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science 321, 1322–1327 (2008).

  29. C. J. MacDonald, K. Q. Lepage, U. T. Eden, H. Eichenbaum, Hippocampal “time cells” bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron 71, 737–749 (2011).

  30. S. M. Polyn, K. A. Norman, M. J. Kahana, A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. Psychol. Rev. 116, 129–156 (2009).

Acknowledgments: This work was sponsored by NIH grant MH-061975, the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) grant SFB 780-TP3, and Federal Ministry of Education and Research (Bundesministerium für Bildung, Wissenschaft, Forschung und Technologie, Germany) grant BCNT TP B3. We are most grateful to J. Stein and H. Urbach for help with localizing electrode locations in postoperative magnetic resonance imaging. We thank the patients and their families for their participation in this research.